‘Twas the night before Christmas, when all through the house. Not a creature was stirring – except for the baby.

Written by Abby O’ Connell. Edited by Tobias Constien.

Ensuring a peaceful night’s sleep for their little ones is very often a top priority for parents, particularly over the festive season. The end of the year typically disrupts a child’s usual routine, which can impact sleep quality: shorter sleep duration, more frequent night waking, or delayed onset of sleep (Mindell & Williamson, 2018). Aside from the benefit of giving parents a moment of peace amidst the festivities, good sleep is critical for infant cognitive development (Fadzil, 2021).

Here at the UCD Babylab, we have been focusing on the importance of sleep research for some time. 

In the past few years, the UCD Babylab has studied sleep specifically in children who have been diagnosed with Tourette syndrome. There is increasing recognition of the high prevalence of sleep issues in children with Tourette syndrome, a condition characterised by motor and vocal tics. In a recently published study from the UCD Babylab, former PhD student Lisa Keenan and colleagues (2024) investigated sleep and daytime functioning in children with Tourette syndrome using an “actiwatch”, which children wore on their wrists for two weeks. She found that relative to healthy children, children with Tourette syndrome had significantly increased time in bed, increased sleep onset latency, reduced sleep efficiency, and lower subjective sleep quality, but comparable actual sleep time. 

Other research from the UCD Babylab also identified the lasting impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on children’s sleep quality. Findings from a systematic review from former lab assistant Eleanor Colreavy and colleagues (2023), suggest that sleep patterns of children with Tourette syndrome may be more impacted by the pandemic than the average child. Given that there is generally more sleep issues reported in children with Tourette syndrome, they recommended further research in relation to the sleep health of children with Tourette syndrome in a post-pandemic era. By identifying sleep issues potentially persisting after COVID-19, the true impact of the pandemic on the sleep of children and adolescents with Tourette syndrome may be ascertained.

Currently in the UCD Babylab, we are working on a number of sleep related studies in typically developing children. One major area of interest for the upcoming year will be the impact of screen-related behaviours or screen-time on sleep and executive functioning. Current guidelines recommend keeping screen-time under one hour per day, ideally alongside an adult, for children aged between 2 and 5 years (American Academy of Pediatrics, 2016). However, as screens become more and more prevalent at home, in the form of smartphones, smart TVs, or tables, the actual usage often exceeds these recommendations. This can also negatively impact children’s sleep quality and their cognitive development, particularly when considering the nature of the viewing content (e.g., fast-paced, fantastical; Essex et al., 2022). These areas are largely unexplored despite their prevalence in the current increasingly digital environment and will be interesting to study in future research.

2025 will be a very busy year for us as we begin to recruit and continue to explore sleep behaviours, so keep an eye on the website and our social pages to learn how you can get involved! For now, we wish you a Happy Christmas and some good night’s sleep for the whole family.

References

American Academy of Pediatrics. (2016). Media and young minds. Pediatrics, 138(5), Article e20162591, https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-2591.

Colreavy, E., Keenan, L., & Downes, M. (2023). The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on sleep in children with Tourette syndrome in Ireland and the United Kingdom. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine19(8), 1485-1493, https://doi.org/10.5664/jcsm.10628.

Essex, C., Gliga, T., Singh, M., & Smith, T. J. (2022). Understanding the differential impact of children’s TV on executive functions: A narrative-processing analysis. Infant Behavior and Development66, Article 101661, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101661.

Fadzil, A. (2021). Factors affecting the quality of sleep in children. Children8(2), Article 122. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8020122

Keenan, L., Bramham, J., Dinca, M., Coogan, A. N., & Downes, M. (2024). Sleep and daytime functioning in children with tourette syndrome: A two-week case-control study with actigraphy and cognitive assessments. Sleep Medicine113, 313-327, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2023.11.1137.

Mindell, J. A., & Williamson, A. A. (2018). Benefits of a bedtime routine in young children: Sleep, development, and beyond. Sleep Medicine Reviews40, 93-108, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2017.10.007.


The Babylab has got something cooking!

Written by Tobias Constien

Recently, we set up our new toy kitchen at the UCD Babylab. Next to computers, eye-trackers, and EEG equipment, children coming to the lab now have space to play, bake, and cook. While adults often view it as an onerous chore, children get excited about cooking – even if it’s “just pretend”. Where does this fascination come from? This blog posts looks at some recent research to find what gets children cooking!

My first job after starting my PhD at the UCD Babylab was to assemble our new toy kitchen. It sparked a lot of conversation among the PhD students about their favourite childhood toys, and since, I've noticed many of them—usually busy writing papers or analysing data—taking breaks to play around with the toy stovetop or microwave.

I remember myself playing in our toy kitchen with my friends when we were children. We would pretend going to the shop, buying groceries, and loading up our fridge with cartons of milk, colourful wooden fruits, or vegetables. No matter how old you are – I bet you remember playing with a toy kitchen as well. Toy kitchens are one of the classic toys for children. They are reported to have been first developed in the middle-ages, providing fun, distraction, and, indeed, opportunities for development to children across centuries.

If you browse through the catalogue of any toy manufacturer, you will likely find toy kitchens of all shapes and sizes. Manufacturers often advertise their specific toys as particularly beneficial to children’s development, yet without the necessary scientific evidence to back up their claims. Recognizing this obscurity in the toy marketplace, the American Academy of Paediatrics recently issued guidance for parents (Healey et al., 2019). Based on cumulative evidence, they say that the specific toy itself does not matter as much to development compared to the social interaction it stimulates between children and their parents, peers, or siblings. In other words, they recommend toys that encourage collaboration. 

In this regard, simple kitchen equipment has been shown to be particularly valuable in eliciting varied social interactions that are rich in language, pretending, cooperation, and creativity. A study from researchers in Australia (Quinn & Kidd, 2019), for example, invited parents and their 18-month-old infants to play either with simple kitchen equipment (i.e., a saucepan, wooden spoon, teacups) or more functional toys (i.e.., a wooden puzzle, drawing board). They found that parents playing with the kitchen equipment were more engaged in their child’s play compared to families solving the puzzle or using the drawing board. As such, the kitchen toys encouraged more interaction and facilitated communication between parents and children, which subsequently can promote further development of language and cognition. In other words, a simple saucepan and wooden ladle may be just as beneficial for children’s development as an expensive, elaborate toy kitchen as long as children can have engaging, exciting, and safe interactions with their parents, peers, or siblings while playing.

Our kitchen at the UCD Babylab also came with the promise of “encouraging development” on its packaging. It features enticing buttons to push, and the stovetop lights up when you put a pot down. Like the researchers in Australia from the example above, we are interested in which aspects of children’s play specifically encourage development. We chose to investigate the value of children’s imagination in play – something that a toy kitchen also encourages. We’ll keep you updated on this research via our website, but for now, we are just excited how children will react to our new toy kitchen!

Further Reading:

  • Read more about my PhD Project on children’s play

We are currently planning the TEDDY study, my PhD project that looks at pretend play and executive functions in toddlerhood. Read more about it here.

  • Tips for Choosing Toys for Toddlers

Rebecca Parlakian from the Zero to Three organization in the States have put together some tips for parents on choosing toys that encourage development for toddlers. Kitchen equipment is on her list! Read more here.

References

Healey, A., Mendelsohn, A., Sells, J. M., Donoghue, E., Earls, M., Hashikawa, A., McFadden, T., Peacock, G., Scholer, S., Takagishi, J., Vanderbilt, D., & Williams, P. G. (2019). Selecting Appropriate Toys for Young Children in the Digital Era. Pediatrics, 143(1). https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2018-3348 

Quinn, S., & Kidd, E. (2019). Symbolic play promotes non-verbal communicative exchange in infant-caregiver dyads. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 37(1), 33-50. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12251

Why is the prefrontal cortex so important?

Why is the prefrontal cortex so important?

Credit: Medium.com

The prefrontal cortex is located in our frontal lobe, the part of our brain at the very front of our head.  The prefrontal cortex is involved in executive functions, or the higher cognitive functions, of our brain. Executive functions act as the chief’s executive officer and include memory, attention, flexibility, planning, and problem solving. For example, the prefrontal cortex is involved in decision making by considering past events and experiences in order to make the best choices. The prefrontal cortex also plays a role in short term memory. It also affects things like holding conversations, reasoning, self-monitoring and time management.

The prefrontal cortex is one of the slowest parts of the brain to develop, only reaching full maturity in our mid-twenties. This can explain why children and teenagers are more prone to risk-taking behaviour, while adults are generally better at planning ahead and reasoning.

Individuals who suffer damage to the prefrontal cortex will often continue to have normal movement abilities and intelligence. However, they frequently display difficulties with executive functions such as memory and attention. Damage to the prefrontal cortex can also cause personality changes, abnormal emotional responses, and difficulty in functioning in daily life.

Phineas Gage is a classic neuropsychological example of the effects of prefrontal cortex damage. Gage was a railroad worker who suffered an accident in which a metal rod was driven through his frontal lobe. He survived the accident, but his friends remarked that his personality changed drastically – he was “no longer Gage”. Recently, it has been debated as to whether the extent of Gage’s transformation has been exaggerated (read more here: https://thepsychologist.bps.org.uk/volume-21/edition-9/phineas-gage-unravelling-myth).

As the prefrontal cortex is linked to so many critical functions – memory, attention, decision making, reasoning – it is clear that it is an important part of our brain, particularly for children. The development of the prefrontal cortex is essential for young children to begin to engage with and navigate the world around them.

Our research looks at attention in children at risk for ADHD. We are currently running a study for babies aged 10-20 months. If you would like more information, or would like to take part, please email sarah.conroy1@ucd.ie or fill out the form on our  homepage!


Looking at attention

ADHD is associated with poor attention control, the markers of which include reduced inhibition and cognitive flexibility. These refer to the capacity to ignore irrelevant or distracting objects, and the ability to disengage attention from one object and focusing on another, respectively. Laboratory tasks designed to measure these cognitive processes typically involve showing an object on a computer screen, along with other distracting objects that are designed to capture participants’ attention.

In our lab, participants’ attention can be observed using an eye-tracker, which allows us to determine the location on the computer screen that is being focused on at any given moment. This is especially useful in infant research as it is unobtrusive and does not require any overt responses from the participant, like pressing buttons or answering questions. During an eye-tracking session, infants are sat either on their parent’s lap or in a highchair, while viewing moving images on a computer screen.

The eye-tracking system, attached below the monitor, records infants’ eye movements while they view the images on the screen.

The eye-tracking system is made up of projectors that shines infrared light (invisible to humans) and cameras that records the reflection of the light on the eyes. This reflection pattern changes slightly as the eyes rotate and move around while looking at different areas of the screen. The eye-tracker is able to use this to calculate the location of focus throughout the session.

A recording of gaze locations being tracked while a participant is looking at a scene

This allows us to collect information such as the pattern of gazes, the amount of time spent looking at specific objects, and even changes in pupil size, which are then used as measures of inhibition and cognitive flexibility.

A (Brief) History of ADHD

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterised by persistent symptoms of inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity. These symptoms must cause significant impairment in social, academic, or occupational functioning. It is prevalent worldwide, with prevalence estimates ranging from 1-5%. Yet how did scientists come to this definition of ADHD? This article will offer a brief glimpse of the history of ADHD, from its early beginnings in the mid-19th century up to today.

One of the earliest reports of ADHD in the literature comes from an anecdotal children’s story by Heinrich Hoffman. Hoffman’s 1884 story is believed to be describing a young boy known as ‘Fidgety Phil’ with ADHD-like symptoms causing a fuss at dinner time.

The 1902 lectures by Sir George Frederic Still are considered by many scientists and researchers to be the scientific starting point of the history of ADHD. Still talked about a defect of moral control in a group of children – who lacked “the control of an action in conformity with the idea of the good of all”. Still also remarked that this particular group of children had no “general impairment of intellect” i.e. they were children of average intelligence with no major deficits. His patients were also reported as having an abnormal incapacity for sustained attention by both parents and teachers. This relates to the current ADHD criteria for ADHD symptoms to be present in more than one setting (for example, at home and at school).

Franz Kramer and Hans Pollnow reported on a hyperkinetic disease in infancy, of which the most characteristic symptom was motor restlessness. Kramer and Pollnow noted that patients had remarkable motor activity which appeared to be very urgent. This can be related to today’s concept of hyperactivity. Patients were also reported to not be able to remain still for prolonged periods of time. This is similar to the American Psychiatric Association’s description of children with ADHD as being “driven by a motor” (2000). Kramer and Pollnow also make references to distractibility, and patients having difficulty completing tasks or concentrating.



Credit: © Rawpixel / Fotolia

In 1937, Charles Bradely reported a positive effect of stimulant medication in children with various behaviour disorders. He discovered that the stimulant Benzedrine was most likely to benefit children with short attention spans, dyscalculia, mood lability, hyperactivity, impulsiveness, and poor memory. As such, this was the first use of stimulant medication as a treatment for ADHD.

Scientists discovered a pattern of hyperactivity being related to reports of brain damage in children. Research in the 1930s and 1940s supported the idea of a causal connection between brain damage and abnormal behaviour. Thus, the idea emerged that minimal brain damage to the brain could cause hyperactive behaviour. Researcher Alfred Strauss considered the symptom of hyperactivity to be a sufficient diagnostic sign of underlying brain damage. Further, Strauss argued that hyperactivity could be able to distinguish between brain-injured and non-brain-injured children.

Criticism began to arise over the necessity of the brain to be damaged in hyperactive children. Laufer and colleagues proposed that there may be a functional disturbance in the brain, rather than damage. Clements (1966) specified the three symptoms of  an inability to control attention, impulsivity, and motor function. Thus, the three core symptoms characterizing ADHD of inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity were established with the definition of minimal brain dysfunction.

From the 1960s, criticism of minimal brain dysfunction arose. It was criticised for being too general. Minimal brain dysfunction was later replaced with more specific labels such as hyperactivity, learning disability, dyslexia, or language disorder. In the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) 2nd edition (a manual used to diagnose most mental illnesses), hyperactivity was referred to as being “characterised by over-activity, restlessness, distractibility, and short attention span” (APA, 1968).

In the 1970s, the focus shifted more to the issues of attention, rather than hyperactivity. This shift was aided by an influential paper by Virginia Douglas in 1972. As a result, the publication of DSM-III in 1980 saw the introduction of a new disorder – attention deficit disorder with or without hyperactivity. The DSM-III contained three separate symptom lists for inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity. In the DSM-III-R (APA, 1987) attention deficit hyperactivity disorder became a single disorder, with all symptoms combined into a single list. The DSM-IV (APA, 1994) divided ADHD into three subtypes: (i) ADHD – inattentive subtype, (ii) ADHD – hyperactive subtype, and (iii) ADHD – combined subtype. It was also in the 1990s that ADHD was recognised as not being an exclusively childhood disorder which disappeared with age. It was instead recognised that ADHD was a chronic persistent disorder remaining into adulthood in many cases. However, it was only with the release of DSM-V (APA, 2013) that ADHD was officially recognised in adults.

How does our research fit in?

The UCD Neuropsychology Lab is currently conducting a study looking at potential early markers for ADHD. We are particularly interested in sleep, sensory processing, and family functioning and their relation with ADHD. For our study, we are recruiting children under 6 who have a parent or older sibling with a diagnosis of ADHD. If you would like to take part, you can click on the ‘How Do I Take Part?’ tab at the top of the screen!



How do we measure sleep?

Since our upcoming project is studying the role of sleep in attention development, one of the most important pieces of new equipment in the lab are our sleep actigraphs. These nifty gadgets record movement and when worn through the night, are able to measure the amount and quality of sleep.

The actigraphs that participants will wear

 

We recently recruited an honorary research assistant to help demonstrate how easy it is to collect the sleep data with the actigraphs and what information can be gleaned from it.


Research Assistant Charlotte inspecting the actigraph before giving it her seal of approval

Using the actigraph is extremely simple. Just put it on the ankle of our infant participants, like you would a watch, and the actigraph will start recording automatically. There are no lights, beeps or buzzes to interfere with regular day-to-day activity and the only input we require is for the grown-ups to note down what time the infants went to bed and got up the next morning.

When the actigraph is brought back to the lab, the data will be downloaded from it and we will be able to chart the sleep patterns, and calculate the quality of sleep.


Charts showing amount of movement during sleep (top), sleep/awake periods (middle), and periods of active sleep phase during the sleep cycle (bottom)

Some of the measures that can be extracted and compared to other participants

One of the aims of the study is to investigate the influence of infant sleep behaviour on attention development in young children and the data collected using the actigraphs is a key component in this. The study hopes to help us better understand attentional disorders such as ADHD and potentially lead to the development of early interventions in the future.